Sunday, April 29, 2012
Homosexuality in animals and humans
http://www.themalaysianinsider.com/opinion/article/it-is-homophobia-that-is-unnatural
-RYAN GUPTA
Thursday, April 19, 2012
Being insecure: how bad is it?
A paper came out by Ein-Dor et al. trying to address a proposed paradox, that individuals who form secure attachments have an evolutionary advantage but almost half of the human species can be classified as insecure in attachments. This group proposed that while insecure individuals have a lower fitness, mixed groups of individuals are better adapted for survival than groups of only secure individuals. Their hypothesis depends on the assumption that these traits evolved in closely related groups, and that forming insecure attachments is always detrimental to the individual. Frankenhuis published a response arguing against this hypothesis, or at least in favor of further testing. According to him, the tendency to form insecure attachments could have developed as an individually advantageous trait in environments where help is less often offered. He argues that there are several holes in Ein-Dor et al.’s hypothesis. For one, they tested homogeneously secure groups and heterogeneous groups, not homogeneously insecure groups. They also did not consider the effect of the kinds of environments where insecure attachment patterns are more prevalent or provide evidence that social groups were comprised of individuals closely related enough to support a group benefit hypothesis. In more simple terms the debate is whether “unhealthy” relationships can be traced back to a time and place in human history where people who tried to create “healthy” relationships were hurt and trust was a disadvantage.
The original paper:
http://pps.sagepub.com/content/5/2/123.short
Commentary by Frankenhuis:
http://www.frontiersin.org/Evolutionary_Psychology/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00172/full
- Nicole Scott
Morality: Where did it come from?
The ant equivalent of our cattle industry
-RYAN GUPTA
Storytelling for survival
http://www.frontiersin.org/Evolutionary_Psychology/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00133/abstract
-Nicole Scott
Wednesday, April 18, 2012
Is Kin Selection Wrong?
In a stunning example of the inevitable marching forward of science, prominent ecologist E.O. Wilson, famed for helping lay the groundwork for modern theories which explain the genesis of social evolution, is now rejecting his own work. Originally, the favored explanation for altruistic behavior - one of the foundations of sociality - was kin selection. Kin selection is an evolutionary theory which asserts organisms can increase their fitness through actions which harm them individually. As long as their actions benefit other organisms which share, or are likely to share their genes, the altruist increases its inclusive fitness.
This was widely supported in the ecological community, because many of its predictions were born out. Organisms which resided in community nests developed sociality faster than organisms which lacked them. Organisms with strong family-rearing instincts formed into colonies of social organisms more frequently than lone wolf species.
However, according to Wilson, all that is circumstantial. In his new paper, Wilson cites group selection as the driving force behind altruism and, by extension, sociality. Group selection differs from kin selection mostly in the level of selection. Kin selection takes place at the genetic level, because inclusive fitness can increase and decrease regardless of which individual organism carries the genes. Even if an allele codes for a suicidally altruistic behavior, if that behavior causes enough relatives to survive and thereby increase the frequency of the gene in the next generation, it is still a beneficial trait. In contrast, group selection takes place at the level of organism group. Competition between groups of organisms puts a separate set of selective pressures on the individuals in the groups, promoting behaviors like selflessness and altruism, because groups made of those types of individuals out-compete groups of selfish jerks. Wilson asserts that in this scenario, high relatedness within groups is a symptom of close proximity for some time, no a cause of group formation in the first place.
Wilson’s new theories are extremely controversial within the scientific community - many ecologists consider him to have lost credibility for suggesting that kin selection is false. As for myself, I don’t think that Dr. Wilson should lose credibility, but I am not sure if I believe his assertions. His theory of group selection does not adequately explain why groups of cooperating organisms formed in the first place. Furthermore, kin selection makes several predictions which group selection does not, which nature has proven to be true. For example, kin selection implies that organisms with higher relatedness should be more social. Lo and behold, order hymenoptera, a haplodiploid gender determinant group, has a higher proportion of eusocial organisms than any other order. Given that one quirk of haplodiploid sex determination is a higher relatedness to one’s siblings than to one’s own children, it is easy to see how this supports kin selection.
You can read E.O. Wilson’s entire paper at http://www.biology.ed.ac.uk/archive/conferences/evolbiol2006/papers/Wilson.pdf and come to your own conclusion about this large debate.
-Adam Geiger
April 18, 2012, 3:11am
- Permalink
Tuesday, April 17, 2012
So That’s Why They Call it Humanity
Biologists have long been fascinated with the interactions between animals, both inter- and intra-species relations, studying everything from parasitisms to mutalisms, sexual selection to family groups to true eusociality. Yet the one constant that remains through the vast amounts of diversity when you get to the root of all actions is selfishness. Every action that every organism willingly performs benefits the actor in some way, otherwise it would be selected out. Every organism, that is, except for humans. We humans have found a way to defy natural selection (wouldn’t Darwin be disgusted?) through acts of compassion.
There is evidence that as early as six million years ago, human ancestors began displaying the first hints of empathy. An article published in Nature titled A Possible Case of Hypervitaminosis A in "Homo erectus" implies that even Homo erectus displayed feelings of compassion by feeding, protecting and caring for a woman who developed a paralyzingly painful condition. DiscoveryNews published an article a year-and-a-half ago that outlines the proposed evolutionary model for the development of human compassion. This is hugely exciting! As far as I can tell, Homo sapiens is the only extant species that forms such strong bonds of friendship and even cares about the general well-being of total strangers. Chimpanzees, our closest living relative, certainly do not exhibit the same kindness. Jane Goodall recounts in her reports the fate of an older male chimp named McGregor who suffered from polio. The paralyzed chimp was shunned, attacked on multiple occasions, and excluded from social activities despite efforts to join.
Perhaps some would consider compassion a frail trait, seeing as how it is only present in one species. They may even say it is maladaptive – after all, it tends to lower fitness. But I say we humans thrive on compassion. It may not be personally beneficial, but it benefits the species as a whole. No other species is capable of such a global perspective. So next time you hear someone talk about ‘losing their faith in humanity’, just remember that compassion has been around for millions of years. I don’t think it’s going anywhere any time soon.
-Elizabeth Richardson
Monday, April 16, 2012
Getting Sick Of It: How Caterpillars Try To Cheat the Group Protection System (and Fail)
Sunday, April 8, 2012
Other species have best friends too!
Monday, April 2, 2012
Eusociality of termites
Is there an evolutionary basis to being nice to old people?
-Mindy MacKay